While I continue researching for my thesis project,
I’m realizing more and more not to disregard ‘old’ articles just because they
are, well, ‘old’. 3 articles by Partridge and colleagues from the 1980s have
proven to be of great use to me in understanding costs associated with mating,
and how choice influences all individuals in the equation (i.e. males, females
and subsequent offspring.) The following are annotations for these articles:
Fowler, K. & Partridge, L. 1989. A cost of
mating in female fruit flies. Nature 228:760-761.
This article looks at the costs a female endures
when mating with a male in the model organism Drosophila melanogaster. This is done through mating experiments
where virgin females were subjected to either a group of males who all were
capable of mating (high-mating) or a group of males where only one of the males
was actually capable of mating (low-mating). Males in the low-mating scenario
still displayed courting behaviours towards females, but had genitalia removed
so they could not actually mate. Results showed that the females who were
exposed to high-mating scenarios had significantly lower life spans then those
exposed to low-mating. Although this may be a consequence of injury, parasites,
or effects of sperm, it still is related to the actual process of mating. These
results therefore have profound effects on the future reproduction success of
these females. These conclusions are beneficial for me to understand in order
to predict then why a female may alter her investment in the subsequent
offspring, depending on the costs she has endured through mating.
In this experiment, larval survival rates are used
as a measure of D. melanogasters’ offspring
fitness. Males and females are exposed to treatments in which they have the
ability to choose mates (cages with many individuals), or where they are unable
to choose their mate (randomly selected female exposed to single randomly
selected male). Results show higher
fitness in subsequent offspring (i.e. more offspring when exposed to competing
larvae) of parents who did have choice in who they mated with, compared to
those who did not. The author discusses possibilities of the genetics of these
individuals influencing choice, and how if there are differences in paternal
and maternal genes, it can lead to fitter offspring because of heterozygosity.
This information is relevant to my research, because it is important to
understand the how the identity of the parents can have a significant affect on
their offsprings’ fitness, especially since the flies in my experiment are
exposed to a no-choice treatment.
Partridge, L. & Farquhar, M. 1981. Sexual
activity reduces lifespan of male fruit flies. Nature 294:580-581.
Partridge and Farquhar study how mating affects the fruit flies’ lifespan by exposing males (Drosophila melanogaster) to different
numbers of receptive females and comparing their longevity with control groups.
Males in experimental groups were exposed to 1-8 virgin females a day, while
males in the control groups were exposed to females who had already been
inseminated and therefore would not remate, or with no females at all. Males
exposed to the highest number of receptive females (i.e. 8 virgins per day) were
shown to have the lowest longevity compared to males who were exposed to fewer
receptive females (slightly higher longevity), and control groups (greatest
longevity). Understanding the costs associated with mating can help us to
understand differences in parental investment of their offspring.
Although these articles are not exactly directly
related to my research, the basis of mate choice and affect on all of the
individuals involved is very useful background knowledge to have. It allows me to
better understand costs of mating behaviours, and hypothesize therefore why
differences in parental investment based on the identity of mates may have
adaptive benefits.
No comments:
Post a Comment