Friday, December 14, 2012
Friday, November 16, 2012
Rinse, lather, rep()
In some of our assays we sort flies by size, and record the number of individuals found in each sieve. When the data is entered, it might look like this (ht Conor Delar)
But how can we actually compare (in R), for instance, the weight of mated and unmated females from the sample? We could do contingency tables to see if the frequency of counts in the two groups, but what if we wanted to compare the mean "girth" of these two groups? You could tediously enter the size of each individual (e.g. for unmated females, 1420,1420,1420,1356,1313,1313 etc...), but there must be a better way
Here, I will show how we can use the rep() function
Where I will specifiy in the first parameter what values I want repeated, and then for the second how many times I want it repeated.
> deLHM2 Assay2 Size.um. Male.Unmated. Female.Unmated. Male.Mated. Female.Mated. 1 LHm (L2) 1420 1 3 1 3 2 LHm (L2) 1365 4 1 2 3 3 LHm (L2) 1313 1 2 1 3 4 LHm (L2) 1262 2 4 0 1 5 LHm (L2) 1214 6 2 3 0 6 LHm (L2) 1167 3 8 3 3 7 LHm (L2) 1122 7 21 0 3 8 LHm (L2) 1079 4 10 3 0 9 LHm (L2) 1038 2 7 0 2 10 LHm (L2) 998 4 8 0 011 LHm (L2) 948 3 6 0 0
But how can we actually compare (in R), for instance, the weight of mated and unmated females from the sample? We could do contingency tables to see if the frequency of counts in the two groups, but what if we wanted to compare the mean "girth" of these two groups? You could tediously enter the size of each individual (e.g. for unmated females, 1420,1420,1420,1356,1313,1313 etc...), but there must be a better way
Here, I will show how we can use the rep() function
Where I will specifiy in the first parameter what values I want repeated, and then for the second how many times I want it repeated.
> deLHmFU <-rep(c(deLHM2$Size.um.),c(deLHM2$Female.Unmated.)) > deLHmFM <-rep(c(deLHM2$Size.um.),c(deLHM2$Female.Mated.)) > deLHmFU [1] 1420 1420 1420 1365 1313 1313 1262 1262 1262 1262 1214 1214 1167 1167 1167 1167 1167 [18] 1167 1167 1167 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 [35] 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1122 1079 1079 1079 1079 1079 1079 1079 1079 1079 1079 [52] 1038 1038 1038 1038 1038 1038 1038 998 998 998 998 998 998 998 998 948 948 [69] 948 948 948 948 > deLHmFM [1] 1420 1420 1420 1365 1365 1365 1313 1313 1313 1262 1167 1167 1167 1122 1122 1122 1038 [18] 1038 >
Now I could proceed with the correct analyses (in this case due to non-normality of distributions, a Mann-Whitney test and a Cliff's delta) to compare the two groups.
Tuesday, October 23, 2012
Male genotype influences female reproductive investment in Drosophila melanogaster
Pischedda,
A., Stewart, A.D., Little, M.K. & Rice, W. 2011. Male genotype influences
female reproductive investment in Drosophila
melanogaster. Proc Biol Sci 278:2165-72.
This
study is the first form of direct evidence that males vary genetically in their
influences on female fecundity, egg sizes and overall female investment in reproduction.
Female Drosophila melanogasters were
mated with males from 10 different ‘worldwide lines’ (to account for genetic
variation) for 2 hours. After this time, females were placed into individual
oviposition vials for 22 hours, followed by transfer into a new oviposition
vial for another 22 hours. After that, all of the females who mated with males
from the same worldwide line were put into the same egg laying chamber and
allowed 4 hours to oviposition. Eggs from the chamber were then photographed
and measured. Results found that a male’s population of origin did not affect
egg size, but did affect females' fecundity in the first 22 hours after mating.
The genotype of the males within a population did however account for some of
the variation seen in egg size. This study is very useful to me, as it relates
directly to the question at the forefront of my research. It does not however
address the mechanisms causing the variations seen, such as female cryptic
choice or manipulations by males, so leaves room for further investigation and
analysis.
Females increase egg deposition in favour of large males...
Evans,
J.P., Box, T.M., Brooshooft, P., Tatler, J.R. & Fitzpatrick, J.L. 2010. Females
increase egg deposition in favour of large males in the rainbowfish, Melanotaenia australis. Behavioural
Ecology 21:465-469.
Through
this study, the researchers address how sexual selection favours flexibility in
maternal investment, using rainbowfish as a model species. Females were
individually placed into large tanks, with both a small and a large male that
were confined to containers within the tank. First, they observed the amount of
time the female spent within one body length of the containers containing the
males. After four days, either the large or the small male was released into
the tank with the female, and they were allowed 4 more days to interact. The
eggs produced by the female during this time were collected, counted and
photographed. Results showed that during the initial 4 days, females spent 70%
of their time within one body length of the large male, and they produced two
times as many eggs when they mated with the large male (large males are phenotypically
preferred). The variation in maternal investment defined within this study is
important for understanding the effect of a mate’s phenotype on maternal
investment, but it fails to address the effect of a mate’s genetic identity.
While I continue researching for my thesis project,
I’m realizing more and more not to disregard ‘old’ articles just because they
are, well, ‘old’. 3 articles by Partridge and colleagues from the 1980s have
proven to be of great use to me in understanding costs associated with mating,
and how choice influences all individuals in the equation (i.e. males, females
and subsequent offspring.) The following are annotations for these articles:
Fowler, K. & Partridge, L. 1989. A cost of
mating in female fruit flies. Nature 228:760-761.
This article looks at the costs a female endures
when mating with a male in the model organism Drosophila melanogaster. This is done through mating experiments
where virgin females were subjected to either a group of males who all were
capable of mating (high-mating) or a group of males where only one of the males
was actually capable of mating (low-mating). Males in the low-mating scenario
still displayed courting behaviours towards females, but had genitalia removed
so they could not actually mate. Results showed that the females who were
exposed to high-mating scenarios had significantly lower life spans then those
exposed to low-mating. Although this may be a consequence of injury, parasites,
or effects of sperm, it still is related to the actual process of mating. These
results therefore have profound effects on the future reproduction success of
these females. These conclusions are beneficial for me to understand in order
to predict then why a female may alter her investment in the subsequent
offspring, depending on the costs she has endured through mating.
In this experiment, larval survival rates are used
as a measure of D. melanogasters’ offspring
fitness. Males and females are exposed to treatments in which they have the
ability to choose mates (cages with many individuals), or where they are unable
to choose their mate (randomly selected female exposed to single randomly
selected male). Results show higher
fitness in subsequent offspring (i.e. more offspring when exposed to competing
larvae) of parents who did have choice in who they mated with, compared to
those who did not. The author discusses possibilities of the genetics of these
individuals influencing choice, and how if there are differences in paternal
and maternal genes, it can lead to fitter offspring because of heterozygosity.
This information is relevant to my research, because it is important to
understand the how the identity of the parents can have a significant affect on
their offsprings’ fitness, especially since the flies in my experiment are
exposed to a no-choice treatment.
Partridge, L. & Farquhar, M. 1981. Sexual
activity reduces lifespan of male fruit flies. Nature 294:580-581.
Partridge and Farquhar study how mating affects the fruit flies’ lifespan by exposing males (Drosophila melanogaster) to different
numbers of receptive females and comparing their longevity with control groups.
Males in experimental groups were exposed to 1-8 virgin females a day, while
males in the control groups were exposed to females who had already been
inseminated and therefore would not remate, or with no females at all. Males
exposed to the highest number of receptive females (i.e. 8 virgins per day) were
shown to have the lowest longevity compared to males who were exposed to fewer
receptive females (slightly higher longevity), and control groups (greatest
longevity). Understanding the costs associated with mating can help us to
understand differences in parental investment of their offspring.
Although these articles are not exactly directly
related to my research, the basis of mate choice and affect on all of the
individuals involved is very useful background knowledge to have. It allows me to
better understand costs of mating behaviours, and hypothesize therefore why
differences in parental investment based on the identity of mates may have
adaptive benefits.
Friday, October 5, 2012
Long Lab Group Photo October 2012
L:R Conor Delar, Heather McLeod, Erin Sonser, Hannah Tennant, Adam Lounsbury, Maya Ashoka, Sahsa Thomsen (Not pictured: Justine Kraemer or Tristan Long)
Saturday, August 18, 2012
Assessing Direct and Indirect Costs
Orteiza, N., Linder, J., & Rice, W. 2005. Sexy sons from re-mating do not recoup the direct costs of harmful male interactions in the Drosophila melanogaster laboratory model system. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 18(5), 1315-1323.
This article explores how female interactions with males can reduce lifetime survival and fecundity, and assesses any indirect benefits as a result. Virgin females were introduced into two conditions simulating constant male exposure and minimal male interaction. Wild-type males and virgin females are housed and allowed to mate, and then half the females are given the chance to re-mate again with brown eyed males. To determine any direct costs to females by re-mating and constant male exposure females were housed with brown-eyed females and would compete for limited yeast. The costs would then be calculated by recording the eye colour and number offspring each female produced. To assess any indirect benefits virgin females were allowed to mate once with a red-eyed male, and then given the chance to re-mate with a brown-eyed male. The male offspring were then separated according to whether their father was an initially mated or re-mated male and cultured to test for paternity. The study found that the grand-offspring of the re-mated male had slightly lower fecundity than the initially mated male and did not make up for the direct costs involved in re-mating caused by increased male exposure. It is estimated that about 10% of a female’s fecundity is lost through re-mating while about 3% is gained through indirect benefits.Monday, August 13, 2012
Sex Ratio in Drosophila melanogaster Populations Effect Male Mating Success
Pavkovic-Lucic, S., Kekic, V., & Cvoro, A. 2009. Larger male mating advantage depends on the sex ratio in Drosophila melanogaster. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 21(2), 155-160.
Friday, August 10, 2012
Effects of Male Size on Mating Success and Female Preference in Drosophila melanogaster
Partridge, L., Ewing, A., Chandler, A. 1987. Male Size and Mating Success in Drosophila melanogaster: The Roles of Male and Female Behavior. Animal Behaviour, 35, 555-562.
In this article the effects of male size in relation to courtship behavior was examined in a non-competitive environment to determine what factors lead to higher courting success in larger males. Male tracking speeds, song frequency and amplitude, as well as frequency of decamping by females was examined. Larger males exhibited louder and more frequent courtship songs as well as faster tracking speeds, while female decamping did not differ significantly among large and small males. This suggests that mating success by larger males is not due to female preference but rather a result of male-male competition. Although the frequency of decamping by the females did not differ, female preference for louder songs and their increased movement as a result of faster tracking speeds by larger males is an important factor in determining female preference for larger males and increased mating success among larger males in a non-competitive environment.
Wednesday, August 1, 2012
Food Cook!
Gradate Students Hannah Tennant and Adam Lounsbury mixing up a delicious batch of Drosophila banana media (yum!)
Tuesday, July 17, 2012
Sieve Sorting
I am sure that ever since you read about the the sieve sorter system mentioned in our paper "A cost of sexual attractiveness to high-fitness females" you have been wondering what you will need to make your own set of sieves? In addition to a set of electo-formed sieves (which we purchased from Precision Electroforming Inc.), and a sieve-shaker (we use a Global Gibson SS-III), you'll need to make some fly-friendly modifications. I have posted some pictures here.
Sieve column on sieve shaker |
Practice Makes Perfect
The last couple of weeks I’ve been getting in some practice
here and there, taking pictures of eggs laid by backup lab culture flies,
trying to figure out how to get the best picture I possibly can for measuring egg size in ObjectJ.
The 'Old' Way |
The 'New' Way |
It wasn’t until the
last cookie that I discovered something that would make the photos much better.
Instead of using a white platform underneath the juice cookie as I had been
doing previously, I tried using a black one. This instantly increased the contrast between
the eggs and the background. I believe that with this discovery, future picture
taking and egg measurements will be much more effective.
Getting in this practice and working out all the little
details now will be very helpful in the future when I start working on my
thesis project.
Sunday, July 15, 2012
Repeatability of mate choice in female red jungle fowl
Johnson, T.S. and Zuk, M. 1995. Behavioural Ecology. 7:245-246
The authors investigated the repeatability of mate choice in female red jungle fowl by examining the heritability of female preference that is an assumption in models explaining the evolution of mate choice. Male morphology was characterized 2 weeks before mate choice trials. For the mate choice trials, males were chosen randomly with the only condition that females never saw a male more than once and males were not paired more than once during the experiment. Females were placed in a small cage in front of two males and left there for 30 min. The female's behaviour was then observed for 20 mins after being released from the small cage. Preference was scored if a female copulated with a male. The results showed that female preference differed with both male trait and the timing of the breeding season. The highest estimate of repeatability was found to be 0.19, which indicates that current heritability may be low. Yet there is still evidence that a heritable component exists in the female red jungle fowl. Females showed repeatability with respect to male combs, but not with respect to hackle feather colour. Male traits can thus evolve through female choice when female preference is genetically determined. and the male trait is heritable.
The authors investigated the repeatability of mate choice in female red jungle fowl by examining the heritability of female preference that is an assumption in models explaining the evolution of mate choice. Male morphology was characterized 2 weeks before mate choice trials. For the mate choice trials, males were chosen randomly with the only condition that females never saw a male more than once and males were not paired more than once during the experiment. Females were placed in a small cage in front of two males and left there for 30 min. The female's behaviour was then observed for 20 mins after being released from the small cage. Preference was scored if a female copulated with a male. The results showed that female preference differed with both male trait and the timing of the breeding season. The highest estimate of repeatability was found to be 0.19, which indicates that current heritability may be low. Yet there is still evidence that a heritable component exists in the female red jungle fowl. Females showed repeatability with respect to male combs, but not with respect to hackle feather colour. Male traits can thus evolve through female choice when female preference is genetically determined. and the male trait is heritable.
Positive genetic correlation between female preference and offspring fitness
Hine, E., Lachish, S., Higgie, M., Blows, M.W. 2002. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 269:2215-2219
These authors showed that female Drosophila serrata prefer extreme male cuticular hydrocarbon (CHs) blends and that this preference affects offspring fitness. Mate choice experiments were preformed using virgin females individually placed with two virgin males. After observed copulation, the males were removed and killed and prepared for gas chromatography analysis of the CHs. From these results, the authors estimated the sexual selection fitness function and linear selection gradient for the more attractive male CH blend. It was then determined if female choice affected offspring fitness with a quantitative genetics experiment. This experiment encompassed female preference, male attractiveness, and offspring fitness all in one. The authors found that female preference is positively correlated with offspring fitness, and that choosing the more attractive male results in genetic benefits. In addition, males with the highest probability of mating conferred intermediate levels of offspring fitness, indicating that female choice in under stabilizing natural selection. Even though male cuticular hydrocarbons experience strong sexual selection, the genes underlying this conferred lower offspring fitness, suggesting a balance between sexual selection and natural selection may have occurred in this population.
These authors showed that female Drosophila serrata prefer extreme male cuticular hydrocarbon (CHs) blends and that this preference affects offspring fitness. Mate choice experiments were preformed using virgin females individually placed with two virgin males. After observed copulation, the males were removed and killed and prepared for gas chromatography analysis of the CHs. From these results, the authors estimated the sexual selection fitness function and linear selection gradient for the more attractive male CH blend. It was then determined if female choice affected offspring fitness with a quantitative genetics experiment. This experiment encompassed female preference, male attractiveness, and offspring fitness all in one. The authors found that female preference is positively correlated with offspring fitness, and that choosing the more attractive male results in genetic benefits. In addition, males with the highest probability of mating conferred intermediate levels of offspring fitness, indicating that female choice in under stabilizing natural selection. Even though male cuticular hydrocarbons experience strong sexual selection, the genes underlying this conferred lower offspring fitness, suggesting a balance between sexual selection and natural selection may have occurred in this population.
Monday, June 25, 2012
Sunday, June 17, 2012
Effects of Drosophila melanogaster Female Size on male Mating Success
Turiegano, E., Monedero, I., Pita, M., Torroja, L., Canal, I. 2012.
Effect of Drosophila melanogaster Female Size on Male Mating Success. J
Insect Behav.
This article examines the importance of female body size on
mating success in Drosophila melanogaster.
The authors first confirm that larger males do mate more rapidly and more
frequently, but stress the importance of examining female size relative to
males. Through observation and analysis, the authors were able to conclude that
in a non-competitive environment, an increase in female size prolongs
copulation latency (i.e. the time between introduction and initiation of
copulation), specifically that larger differences in size between a male and a
female causes an increase in copulation latency. Larger females were also found
to display lengthened avoidance behaviour during courtship. In competitive
environments, it was found that the first male to initiate courtship had a much
higher probability of mating, and that an increase in female size reduces the likelihood
of the larger male initiating courtship. This study is significant because examines the
importance of the relative size of both males and females in determining mating
success, and leaves room for further research into this topic.
Effect of Drosophila melanogaster Female Size on Male Mating Success
Turiegano, E., Monedero, I., Pita, M., Torroja, L., & Canal, I. 2012. J Insect Behav.
The authors examined the effect of female Drosophila melanogaster body size on mating success. Their results confirmed that larger males have a higher mating success. The authors also found that female size with respect to male size also affects mating success; previously the role of female size was unknown. The effect of female size on mating behaviour and dynamics appears to influence male courtship displays. Males show an increased time in courtship initiation towards larger females, larger females are the recipients of more wing vibrations, and larger females tend to avoid males for longer periods of time during courtship attempts. Large males are known to copulate more rapidly in non-competitive experiments. In addition, large males are favoured to initiate courtship in a competitive environment.The analysis of these behaviours confirms that the female is the most important in courtship opportunities and that both male and female body size is important for successful mating.
The authors examined the effect of female Drosophila melanogaster body size on mating success. Their results confirmed that larger males have a higher mating success. The authors also found that female size with respect to male size also affects mating success; previously the role of female size was unknown. The effect of female size on mating behaviour and dynamics appears to influence male courtship displays. Males show an increased time in courtship initiation towards larger females, larger females are the recipients of more wing vibrations, and larger females tend to avoid males for longer periods of time during courtship attempts. Large males are known to copulate more rapidly in non-competitive experiments. In addition, large males are favoured to initiate courtship in a competitive environment.The analysis of these behaviours confirms that the female is the most important in courtship opportunities and that both male and female body size is important for successful mating.
Friday, June 15, 2012
Annotated Bibliography
Turiegano E, Monedero I, Pita M,
Torroja L, Canal I. 2012. Effect of Drosophila
melanogaster Female Size on Male Mating Success. J Insect Behav.
This article considers the
importance of female size when examining male mating success. Previous studies
have revealed that larger males tend to have higher mating success, but did not
take female size into account. The study confirmed that male size affects
mating success, but stresses that this must always be considered relative to
female size. Males show greater latency to courtship toward larger females, larger
females receive more wing vibrations from males during courtship, and larger females
move around more during courtship. The article discusses possible reasons for
observed behaviours, and invites further study into the relation of female and
male size variation toward mating behaviour.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)